Volume 12, Issue 2 (Spring 2024)                   PCP 2024, 12(2): 109-122 | Back to browse issues page


XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Mohajeri Aval N, Narimani M, Sadeghi G, Hajloo N. Comparing the Effectiveness of Mindfulness Meta-Cognitive Therapy vs Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation in People With General Anxiety Disorder. PCP 2024; 12 (2) :109-122
URL: http://jpcp.uswr.ac.ir/article-1-916-en.html
1- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Mohaghegh Ardabili University, Ardabil, Iran. , nastaranmohajeri@yahoo.com
2- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Mohaghegh Ardabili University, Ardabil, Iran.
Full-Text [PDF 664 kb]   (172 Downloads)     |   Abstract (HTML)  (1375 Views)
Full-Text:   (106 Views)
Introduction
One of the severe and common mental health problems is related to anxiety disorder, and every year, it imposes enormous costs on society (Mash & Woolf, 2012). People with general anxiety disorder (GAD) have uncontrollable worries which last at least six months. In addition, people with GAD have other symptoms, such as fatigue, low concentration, irritability, and sleep problems (DeMartini et al., 2019). One of the psychological components in people suffering from anxiety disorders is a defect in emotion regulation strategies (Rabie et al., 2013).
Individuals with GAD usually avoid their painful feelings and negative thoughts and try to suppress them and stay away from doing many of their activities. This negative reaction leads to experiential avoidance (Hayes-Skelton et al., 2013). Experiential avoidance involves an unwillingness to endure upsetting emotions, thoughts, memories, and other private experiences. Such unwillingness leads to maladaptive efforts to resist, escape, and avoid distressing and disturbing experiences in people’s lives (Hayes et al., 1996). Past research has shown that experiential avoidance is the cause of anxiety disorders, especially GAD (Hayes et al., 1996; Orsillo & Roemer, 2005). Although experiential avoidance alleviates distress in the short term, its prolonged use underlies a variety of dysfunctional conditions in adults, including avoidant coping styles, excessive thought suppression, drug or alcohol use to escape from unwanted moods, and avoidance of feared objects, places, or situations (Chawla & Ostafin, 2007).
Evidence shows that negative emotion regulation strategies are related to anxiety disorders (Ghasempour, 2012). Higher emotional reactivity in GAD disorder makes it difficult to regulate emotions, and this fosters complications by making it challenging to identify and understand emotions (Mennin, 2004). Emotion regulation refers to the processes that influence the intensity, duration, and expression of emotions (Gross & Thompson, 2007), and effective emotion regulation can reduce the intensity of negative emotional responses in anxiety-provoking situations. Research has shown a strong relationship between the emotional problems of people of different ages and self-blame, catastrophizing, and rumination as cognitive emotion regulation strategies (Garnefski & Kraaij, 2006). This issue shows that people’s vulnerability to negative and uncomfortable events increases with negative strategies of cognitive emotion regulation (Garnefski et al., 2002).
Anxious people have problems in cognitive processing because anxiety strongly affects cognition (Bar-Haim et al., 2011). The level of anxiety of a person affects the level of cognitive processing of emotional information. Also, anxiety reduces the activity of the prefrontal cortex, and as a result, it causes disturbances in attention (Bishop, 2009) and a lack of control during emotional processing (Osinsky et al., 2012).
Meta-cognitive therapy is a meta-diagnostic therapy and shows that psychopathology is related to cognitive attentional syndrome (CAS) (Wells, 2009). CAS is defined as rumination, ineffective coping styles such as thought suppression, avoidance, repetitive worries, and threat-oriented attention. A more recent one that seems to look specifically at GAD is meta-cognitive therapy (Wells, 2009). Positive and negative meta-cognitive beliefs influence CAS. People with positive meta-cognitive beliefs maintain that having these thoughts is beneficial, and this issue increases a series of repeated negative thoughts. When people engage in repetitive thoughts, negative meta-cognitive beliefs are activated. People conclude that their repetitive thoughts are uncontrollable and harmful at this stage. They begin to worry about their thoughts. A vicious cycle is created where people cannot control their thoughts, no matter how hard they try.
Mindfulness meta-cognitive therapy (MCT) was derived from the meta-cognitive model proposed by Efklides (2008). Efklid’s model was in harmony with the cognitive and meta-cognitive processes used in mindful information processing. This method expresses the mindfulness state as originally proposed by Kabat-Zinn. This method helps to understand the process in a person’s mind in mindfulness. Meta-cognition is used in the classical definition of mindfulness (Kabat-Zinn, 2003). In this definition, the conscious mind is defined as a form of consciousness caused by a change in the content of consciousness and includes thoughts, feelings, images, and perceptions. Mindful people are familiar with metacognition, and this issue reduces the possibility of anxiety and depression recurrence (Teasdale et al., 2002; Watkins et al., 2000). As a result, mindfulness can be conceptualized in terms of meta-cognition (Jankowski & Holas, 2014).
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) transmits mild electric current continuously to the scalp and applies a neuro-modulatory effect. Anodal tDCS increases, and cathodal tDCS decreases cortical excitability (Liebetanz et al., 2002). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has shown that positive emotional stimulation, compared to negative emotional stimulation, can increase the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) (Dolcos et al., 2004).
The prefrontal cortex plays an important role in regulating positive and negative emotions due to its connection with subcortical regions, such as the amygdala and the nucleus accumbens (Sotres-Bayon et al., 2010). Specifically, when a person is involved in negative emotions, dlPFC is one of the brain’s regions involved in processing (Davidson et al., 2000). Research shows that left dlPFC activity is involved in anxiety regulation (Bishop, 2007). Also, findings demonstrate that dlPFC tDCS of the left anodal region causes people to show less emotional reaction to negative content (Peña-Gómez et al., 2011), facilitates attentional control for purposeful cognition (Martin et al., 2013), and attentional vigilance reduces to threat (Heeren et al., 2017).
This study examines the effect of MCT on experiential avoidance, negative emotion regulation strategies, and emotional processing in people with GAD, investigates the impact of tDCS on experiential avoidance, negative emotion regulation strategies, and emotional processing in people with GAD, and compares MCT and tDCS on experiential avoidance, negative emotion regulation strategies, and emotional processing in people with GAD. Based on previous studies, these two treatments are effective for GAD; however, this study compares the two treatment and their effects. In addition, this research investigates which variables have a better impact on these two treatment methods.

Materials and Methods
This was a semi-experimental study with experimental and control groups with a pre-test/post-test design. The statistical population of this study included subjects with GAD in Tehran, Iran, who were referred to Tehran clinics in 2019-2020. The sample consisted of 45 patients with GAD, of which 15 people were in the group under the treatment with MCT, 15 were in the group under the treatment of tDCS, and 15 were in the control group. People were randomly divided into three groups using stratified randomization considering gender. The sample size was selected, considering this is an experimental study and is also similar to previous research. The selection of subjects was purposeful, and they were treated according to the following criteria. The admission criteria were having GAD according to The diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edition, not undergoing any psychotherapy for the past three months, no infection or other psychiatric disorders, no substance abuse, having 18 to 50 years of age, and having at least a high school diploma level. Meanwhile, the exclusion criteria were failure to meet the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edition diagnostic criteria for GAD, having a history of epilepsy and mental retardation, and taking anxiety medications during the study. Clients with GAD, who were evaluated by a generalized anxiety disorder-7 (GAD-7) questionnaire and scored 10 or higher, were included in the sample. Also, 40% (n=18) of the sample were female, and 60% (n=27) were male, with a Mean±SD age of 37.56±4.46 years. All samples were from Iran.
The therapist was the first author of this article. The therapist was trained in comprehensive MCT and tDCS treatment workshops separately.

Study instruments

Generalized anxiety disorder questionnaire

GAD-7 is a 7-item questionnaire designed to evaluate the level of anxiety of patients in the last two previous weeks. The items enquire about the amount of nervousness and anxiety that bothers the patient, the patient’s inability to control and stop their worries, worrying too much about different things, difficulty in calming themselves, being restless for fear of something happening, getting angry, feeling afraid easily, and easily collapsing (Spitzer et al., 2006).
The scores are then totaled and presented from 0 to 21 (based on a 3-point Likert scale). The 5, 10, and 15 scores represent cut-off points for mild, moderate, and severe anxiety, respectively. This questionnaire had good internal consistency (Cronbach α=0.79–0.91). In the Iranian sample, the validity of the correlation between this scale and trait anxiety inventory and anxiety subscale of symptom checklist-90 were 0.71 and 0.63, respectively. Also, the Cronbach α coefficient for the research sample was 0.85 (Naeinian et al., 2011).

Acceptance and action questionnaire–II
The acceptance and action questionnaire-II (AAQ-II) (Bond et al., 2011) contains 10 questions. Each question evaluates the person based on a 7-point Likert scale. The range of AAQ-II score is from 10 to 70. Higher scores indicate more acceptance and less avoidance. The Dutch AAQ-II (Jacobs et al., 2008) showed good internal consistency (Cronbach α=0.85). The Cronbach α of the Persian version of this questionnaire for the general population is 0.82, and for the GAD group is 0.84. The validity of the correlation between this scale and general health questionnaire-28, Beck anxiety inventory, and Beck depression inventory were -0.62, -0.44, and -0.59, respectively (Abasi et al., 2013).

Cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire
The cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire (CERQ) measures the cognitive strategies of emotion regulation. It evaluates the reaction of people facing stressful and life-threatening events (Garnefski et al., 2002). CERQ assesses 9 subscales in 36 questions. Each subscale contains 4 items. Meanwhile, its 9 subscales are catastrophizing, rumination, positive refocusing, positive reappraisal, self-blame, other-blame, acceptance, and planning. This study examined negative cognitive emotion regulation strategies (self-blame, other-blame, rumination, catastrophizing). This questionnaire measures cognitive emotion regulation strategies based on a 5-point Likert scale. The scoring degree is from almost never (1) to almost always (5). Previous research has shown that in this questionnaire, all subscales have good internal consistency (0.68-0.86) (Garnefski et al., 2002). In the Iranian sample, the reliability of the questionnaire using the Cronbach α coefficient for all cognitive scales is reported at 0.76. The validity of this questionnaire was obtained by the factor analysis method (KMO=0.82) (Hasani, 2010).

Emotional processing scale
The emotional processing scale (EPS) examines emotional processing styles and includes 38 items. This questionnaire was created to measure emotional processing and has changed over time so that therapists can use it (Baker et al., 2007). It uses a 10-point Likert scale, and higher scores indicate more problems in emotional processing. This questionnaire includes 8 subscales. Accordingly, 4 subscales are related to the control mechanism of experience and emotional expression (suppression, uncontrolled, dissociation, avoidance). One subscale is related to insufficient emotional processing (intrusion), and the other three are related to capturing emotional experience (lack of attunement, externalized, discordant). Good internal validity has been reported for EPS. This scale’s Cronbach α coefficient and test-retest reliability were reported at 0.92 and 0.79, respectively (Baker et al., 2007). In the Iranian sample, the Cronbach α coefficient for the whole scale and subscales are calculated at 0.95 and 0.85, respectively. The correlation coefficient of this scale with the emotion regulation scale was -0.054 (P>0.01) (Lotfi et al., 2013).

Mindfulness meta-cognitive therapy
All subjects in the meta-cognitive therapy group underwent 8 sessions of MCT. Individual treatment was performed on 15 members of the meta-cognitive therapy group in eight 1-h sessions (one session per week). The sessions were conducted in person. At first, case formulation was performed, and the participants were acquainted with this model. Next, positive and negative meta-cognitive beliefs were identified. According to MCT, attention training, detached mindfulness, and free association techniques were used for treatment in sessions.
The following items were practiced during MCT sessions (Nazaribadie et al., 2020):

Transcranial direct-current stimulation
This research applied direct electrical current to the subject’s scalp through a battery that transmits a constant current. This electrical current was transmitted through 5×7 cm sponge electrodes soaked in salt (Neuroconn, GmbH, ilmenau, Germany). Electrodes were placed on the subjects’ heads according to the 10-20 system. During treatment, the anode was placed on the left prefrontal (F3), and the cathode was placed on the right prefrontal (F4). We used bilateral stimulation over the dlPFC in this study. Right, dlPFC and left dlPFC were the only targeted regions in the previous studies that aimed to regulate anxiety disorders using tDCS. Also, in some studies, excitatory stimulation of the left dlPFC (Peña-Gómez et al., 2011) and, in others, inhibitory stimulation over the right dlPFC (De Raedt et al., 2010) were effective in reducing negative emotions. As a result, in this study, we examined bilateral stimulation, which might enhance the efficacy of interventions. Active tDCS consisted of a constant current of 2 mA for 20 min in 15 sessions. Individual treatment sessions were performed on 15 subjects of the tDCS therapy group. It was a 5-week intervention (three sessions per week). All subjects were told to sit quietly and regulate their breathing.

Study procedures
First, the subjects were selected via the purposeful sampling method. They were assessed using the GAD-7 questionnaire (participants who scored 10 or higher), and their degree of compliance with the inclusion and exclusion criteria was determined. All subjects (n=45) were evaluated by AAQ-II, CERQ, and EPS (pre-test). In the next step, the subjects were randomly assigned into two treatment groups, namely MCT and tDCS, and a control group using stratified randomization considering gender (each group consisted of 15 subjects). The MCT group received 8 sessions of therapy once a week. The tDCS group received fifteen sessions of therapy three times a week. The first author of this article conducted therapeutic sessions in Tehran, Iran. Finally, after treatment, a post-test was done. The post-test consisted of AAQ-II, CERQ, and EPS (the raters who completed the pre-and post-test assessments were blinded to the study). Then, the results were analyzed using the SPSS software, version 19. The subjects were fully aware of the research, and all ethical considerations were observed.

Analytic plan
In this study, according to the type of variables and dimensions of the study to investigate the therapeutic effects of MCT and tDCS, analysis of covariance was used to compare variables related to anxiety before and after treatment (experiential avoidance, negative emotion regulation strategies, and emotional processing). The effectiveness of MCT and tDCS were independent variables, and the degree of experiential avoidance, negative emotion regulation strategies, and emotional processing measured after treatment (post-test) were dependent variables. The covariate variables were the pre-test (AAQ-II, CERQ, and EPS) that calculated the experiential avoidance, negative emotion regulation strategies, and emotional processing before treatment. Anxiety intensity (having a score of 10 or higher in GAD-7) was also controlled.
To evaluate and compare the effects of the two treatment methods, multivariate analysis of covariance and the Tukey test was used for analysis. The Tukey test was used to compare pairs. First, MCT and tDCS were compared for the experiential avoidance variable. Second, the two treatments were compared for the negative emotion regulation strategies variable. Third, they were compared for the emotional processing variable.

Results
In this study, 40% (n=18) of the sample were female, while 60% (n=27) were male, with a Mean±SD age of 37.56±4.46 years.
The effect of mindfulness meta-cognitive therapy and transcranial direct-current stimulation on general anxiety disorder
Table 1 presents the results of a multivariate analysis of covariance to evaluate the effect of MCT and tDCS on experiential avoidance, negative strategies of cognitive emotion regulation, and emotional processing in people with GAD.



According to Table 2, the results of the Pillais trace, Wilks lambda, Hoteling trace, and Roy largest root at a significance level of 0.0005 equal 4.704, 6.314, 6.978, and 22.3, respectively. These results showed that MCT and tDCS were effective in experiential avoidance, negative strategies of cognitive emotion regulation, and emotional processing in people with GAD. In other words, the results indicate a significant difference between MCT, tDCS, and control groups, at wleast in one of the variables of experiential avoidance, negative strategies of cognitive emotion regulation, and emotional processing.



According to the results of Table 3, the effect of MCT and tDCS on the variables of experiential avoidance, negative strategies of cognitive emotion regulation, and emotional processing at the level of significance 0.0005, according to F, are 98.531, 102.263, and 121.26, respectively. Considering the η2, 74.4% of the changes in experiential avoidance, 82.4% in negative strategies of cognitive emotion regulation, and 93.1% of changes in emotional processing are due to MCT and tDCS.



Couple comparison of mindfulness meta-cognitive therapy and transcranial direct-current stimulation
Table 4 shows no significant difference in the experiential avoidance variable between the treatment methods, MCT, and tDCS, according to the mean difference (0.735) and the significance level (P=0.575). There is a significant difference between the MCT and control groups, with a mean difference of 3.66 and a significance level of P=0.003. In addition, there is a significant difference between the tDCS and control groups, with a mean difference of 5.23 and a significant level of P=0.002 (P≤0.05). In other words, both treatments significantly affected the experiential avoidance of GAD (according to the mean difference between MCT and the control group, which is 3.66, while the mean difference between tDCS and the control group is 5.23).



According to Table 5, no significant difference exists in the self-blame, other-blame, and catastrophizing variables between MCT and tDCS, according to the mean difference (1.26, 0.37, 0.265) and the level of significance (P≤0.05). There is a significant difference between the MCT and control groups in the self-blame, other-blame and catastrophizing variables (P=0.005, 0.002, 0.002). Also, there is a significant difference between the tDCS and control group in the self-blame, other-blame, and catastrophizing (P=0.006, 0.003, 0.002).



According to Table 5, there is a significant difference in the rumination variable between the treatment methods, MCT, and tDCS, according to the mean difference (4.63) and the significance level (P=0.008). There is a significant difference between the MCT and the control groups, with a mean difference of (4.741) and a significance level of P=0.003. There is no significant difference between the tDCS and control groups, with a mean difference of 0.276 and a significance level of P=0.224 (P≤0.05). In other words, MCT treatment significantly affected the rumination with GAD, but tDCS had no significant impact on the rumination.
According to Table 5, there is no significant difference in the negative strategies of cognitive emotion regulation variable between the two treatment methods (MCT and tDCS) according to the mean difference of 0.124 and the significance level (P=0.410). There is a significant difference between the MCT and control groups, with a mean difference of 10.394 and a significance level of P=0.007. In addition, there is a significant difference between the tDCS and control groups, with a mean difference of 9.974 and a significance level of P=0.006 (P≤0.05). In other words, both treatments had a significant effect on the negative strategies of cognitive emotion regulation with GAD.
Table 6 shows no significant difference in the emotional processing between MCT and tDCS, according to the mean difference (0.731) and the significance level (P=0.134). There is a significant difference between the MCT and control groups, with a mean difference of 7.80 and a significant level of P=0.000. In addition, there is a significant difference between the tDCS and control groups, with a mean difference of 9.01 and a significant level of P=0.000 (P≤0.05). In other words, both treatments had a significant effect on the emotional processing with GAD (according to the mean difference between MCT and control group which is 7.80 while the mean difference between tDCS and control group is 9.01).



Discussion
Although there is considerable evidence that MCT and tDCS are effective in GAD, this study is the first to compare the effectiveness of these two treatments and the variables each treatment method affects. Consequently, it is imperative to understand better the difference in effectiveness of the two treatment and their implications. The results indicate that both treatments (MCT and tDCS) are effective in experiential avoidance and emotional processing; however, in the rumination subscale, only the MCT treatment method has been effective. Therefore, in the negative strategies of cognitive emotion regulation variable, MCT treatment has been more effective than tDCS. The differences observed can be accounted for by the different focus and techniques used in these therapies. Thus, the specific elements in each therapy seem to play an important role.
Experiential avoidance is a person’s avoidance of painful events, experiences, thoughts, feelings, and memories (Hayes et al., 2004). As its useful counterpart, mindfulness is non-judgmental, purposeful attention in the present moment (Kabat-Zinn, 1990). Many types of research show that mindfulness reduces anxiety because mindfulness reduces experiential avoidance (a person is less likely to avoid unpleasant experiences), and this helps control anxiety (McCluskey et al., 2022). Mindfulness and experiential avoidance represent constructs that influence the experience of emotions (Dimidjian & Linehan, 2003). Negative reinforcement and avoidance processes contribute extensively to the etiology and maintenance of many clinical disorders. MCT emphasizes acceptance (this treatment prevents clients from experiential avoidance) (Segal et al., 2002). There is an inverse relationship between mindfulness and experiential avoidance, and there is a direct relationship between anxiety and experiential avoidance. As a result, the conscious mind reduces anxiety (McCluskey et al., 2022).
Considerable changes in people’s anxiety in the last year of their lives can be shown with the cognitive strategies of rumination, catastrophizing, self-blame, and positive reappraisal strategy. Evidence shows a strong relationship between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and symptoms of emotional problems. The relationships between the use of the cognitive emotion regulation strategies of self-blame, catastrophizing, and rumination and the report of anxiety symptoms, in particular, suggests that the presence of such symptoms might indicate the presence of maladaptive strategies of cognitive emotion regulation (Garnefski & Kraaij, 2007). Techniques that modify cognitive emotion regulation patterns are also used in MCT.
Detached mindfulness causes people to distance themselves from their thoughts and creates a metacognitive awareness of internal events. MCT is effective in reducing anxiety. The use of metaphor is one of the techniques of detached mindfulness. For example, people are taught to visualize their thoughts written on clouds that are moving away (Normann et al., 2014).
MCT focuses on reducing anxiety by modifying meta-cognitive beliefs about the dangerousness and uncontrollability of emotions. MCT focuses on meta-cognitive responses, such as choosing to let go of thoughts (Nordahl et al, 2018).
According to the research conducted by Hoffart et al. and by examining treatment interaction on a measure of the CAS, they concluded that MCT significantly affects positive and negative meta-cognitive beliefs (Hoffart et al., 2018). Another evaluation of the same study reported that negative automatic thoughts were reduced during treatment, suggesting that MCT was effective in improving meta-cognitions (Johnson et al., 2018). The analysis of self-report measures of negative automatic thoughts showed that most participants experienced an improvement in their symptoms at post-treatment and follow-ups.
The increase in anxiety was related to the rise in the left anterior and posterior cingulate of the right insula and the activation of the amygdala. tDCS by increasing dlPFC activity might affect autonomy by modulating the neural activity of subcortical structures such as the amygdala. Also, multiple evidence has shown that the reduction and upregulating of negative emotions are associated with increased dlPFC activity (Eippert et al 2007). Another study investigated that amygdala activity may cause negative reappraisal in the individual (Urrey et al., 2006). dlPFC does not directly influence amygdala activity related to emotional feelings, as there is no direct connection between the two brain regions. It is indirectly through the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, which is directly connected to dlPFC and amygdala (Hartley & Phelps, 2010). Thus, tDCS can reduce experiential avoidance by indirectly affecting the amygdala.
When a person focuses on the external environment, the dlPFC is a central area for cognitive processing (Miller & Cohen, 2001). In particular, dlPFC is one of the critical areas of the prefrontal cortex that plays a role in the cognitive control of emotional mechanisms (Ochsner & Gross, 2005). dlPFC is activated when a person has a conflict in simultaneously performing cognitive and emotional tasks (Ochsner et al., 2009). Since anodal tDCS increases the activity of the underlying cortices, it improves attention compared to emotion processing (Fregni et al., 2005).
Research has shown that people with anxiety disorders have problems managing their emotions (Amstadter, 2008). Recent findings show that left frontal tDCS can influence the reaction rate to negative content, and this issue displays that tDCS is effective in negative cognitive emotion regulation strategies in people with anxiety disorders (Marques et al., 2018). De Raedt et al. (2017) found that left dlPFC tDCS helped reduce spontaneous self-rumination. This research also shows that stimulation of the left dlPFC can reduce negative cognition (De Raedt et al., 2017).
Researches show a functional connection between prefrontal cortex areas and related subcortical areas, such as the amygdala, which allows for the modulation of threat-related structures (Ironside et al., 2019). Due to the effect of regional stimulation on brain networks, the impact of non-invasive brain stimulation methods on cortical areas is greater than on subcortical areas (Polania et al., 2012). However, the evidence shows that tDCS, by stimulating the dlPFC area, can also affect the activation and connection of subcortical areas (Polania et al., 2012; Weber et al., 2014).
The excitatory stimulation of the left dlPFC is used because of this region’s association with reducing negative emotion (Peña-Gómez et al., 2011) and increasing positive emotion (Ollendick et al., 2002). Also, the evidence shows that the inhibitory stimulation of the right dlPFC decreases people’s reactions to negative emotions (De Raedt et al., 2010). The right dlPFC and left dlPFC were the only targeted areas in the previous studies that have been conducted using tDCS to improve anxiety disorders. Even bilateral stimulation is suggested as a research proposal in the article of Vicario et al (2019). Yet, in this study, we investigated bilateral right and left dlPFC stimulation to increase the effectiveness of interventions.

Conclusion
MCT is a promising treatment in terms of acceptability and effectiveness on anxiety symptoms for GAD. In addition, our findings show that bilateral brain stimulation using tDCS in the left and right dlPFC regions reduces negative emotions. The results of our research show that dlPFC plays a vital role in regulating emotions in people with GAD. In conclusion, both MCT and tDCS were effective in GAD. However, MCT had a better outcome in reducing negative cognitive emotion regulation strategies in GAD. In future research, it is essential to compare the effects of these two treatments in follow-up over time.

Study limitations
There are also some significant limitations to consider. This study was limited to examining the effectiveness of mindfulness meta-cognitive therapy and tDCS on the psychological consequences of GAD. Evaluating the effectiveness of these two treatments on other outcomes of GAD, such as social and behavioral outcomes, can be another area of further research. GAD affects the level of irritability and social isolation. As a result, it disrupts the social relationships of people with GAD. Considering social and behavioral factors in future research better demonstrates the effectiveness of these two treatments. The second notable limitation is that due to the application of technology in the tDCS, the effect of hope for new and technological treatments could affect the treatment outcome. Future studies, including the placebo group, could eliminate the impact of psychological indoctrination. The third limitation that could potentially influence the results is the difference in duration of treatment. MCT treatment lasts longer than tDCS treatment, and this issue can affect the treatment result. In future studies, it is better to control the duration of treatment between the two treatment groups.

Ethical Considerations

Compliance with ethical guidelines

All ethical principles were observed in this article. The participants were aware of the research process. They were also assured about the confidentiality of their information, and the research results would be available to them if desired. This study was approved by the Ardabil University of Medical Sciences (Code: IR.ARUMS.REC.1397.093).

Funding
The paper was extracted from the PhD dissertation of Nastaran Mohajeri Aval, approved by Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Mohaghegh Ardabili University.

Authors' contributions
All authors contributed equally to preparing this article.

Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments
The authors thank all the participants who contributed to the study.





References
Abasi, E., Fti, L., Molodi, R., & Zarabi, H. (2012). [Psychometric properties of Persian Version of Acceptance and Action Questionnaire -II (Persian)]. Psychological Methods and Models, 3(10), 65-80. [Link]
Amstadter, A. (2008). Emotion regulation and anxiety disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(2), 211–221. [DOI:10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.02.004] [PMID]
Bar-Haim, Y., Morag, I., & Glickman, S. (2011). Training anxious children to disengage attention from threat: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 52(8), 861–869. [DOI:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02368.x] [PMID]
Baker, R., Thomas, S., Thomas, P. W., & Owens, M. (2007). Development of an emotional processing scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 62(2), 167-178. [DOI:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.09.005] [PMID]
Bishop, S. J. (2009). Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 12(1), 92–98.[DOI:10.1038/nn.2242] [PMID]
Bishop, S. J. (2007). Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: An integrative account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 307-316. [DOI:10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.008] [PMID]
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Guenole, N., & Orcutt, H. K., et al. (2011). Preliminary Psychometric Properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II: A revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behavior Therapy, 42(4), 676-688. [DOI:10.1016/j.beth.2011.03.007] [PMID]
Chawla, N., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Experiential avoidance as a functional dimensional approach to psychopathology: An empirical review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63(9), 871–890.[DOI:10.1002/jclp.20400] [PMID]
Davidson, R. J., Putman, K. M., Larson, C. L. (2000). Dysfunction in neural circuitry of emotion regulation-a possible prelude to violence. Science, 289(5479), 591–594. [DOI:10.1126/science.289.5479.591] [PMID]
DeMartini, J., Patel, G., & Fancher, T. L. (2019). Generalized anxiety disorder. Annals of Internal Medicine, 170(7), ITC49–ITC64.[DOI:10.7326/AITC201904020] [PMID]
De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., Baeken, C., Van Schuerbeek, P., Luypaert, R., & Vanderhasselt, M. A., et al. (2010). Neurocognitive effects of HF-rTMS over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on the attentionalprocessing of emotional information in healthy women: an event-related fMRI study. Biological Psychology, 85(3), 487-495. [DOI:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.015] [PMID]
De Raedt, R., Remue, J., Loeys, T., Hooley, J. M., & Baeken, C. (2017). The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex on implicit self-esteem is mediated by rumination after criticism. Behavior Research and Therapy, 99, 138-146. [DOI:10.1016/j.brat.2017.10.009] [PMID]
Dimidjian, S., & Linehan, M. M. (2003). Defining an agenda for future research on the clinical application of mindfulness practice. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 166–171. [DOI:10.1093/clipsy.bpg019]
Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2004). Dissociable effects of arousal and valence on prefrontal activity indexing emotional evaluation and subsequ¨ent memory: An event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 23(1), 64–74. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.015] [PMID]
Efklides, A. (2008). Metacognition: Defining its facets and levels of functioning in relation to self-regulation and co-regulation. European Psychologist, 13(4), 277-287. [DOI: 10.1027/1016-9040.13.4.277]
Eippert, F., Veit, R., Weiskopf, N., Erb, M., Birbaumer, N., & Anders, S. (2007). Regulation of emotional responses elicited by threat-related stimuli. Human Brain Mapping, 28(5), 409–423.[DOI:10.1002/hbm.20291] [PMID]
Fregni, F., Boggio, P. S., Nitsche, M., Bermpohl, F., Antal, A., & Feredoes, E., et al. (2005). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working memory. Experimental Brain Research, 166(1), 23–30. [DOI:10.1007/s00221-005-2334-6] [PMID]
Garnefski, N., Van Den Kommer, T., Kraaij, V., Teerds, J., Legerstee, J., & Onstein, E. (2002). The relationship between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and emotion problems: Comparison between a clinical and a non-clinical sample. European Journal of Personality, 16(5), 403-420.[DOI:10.1002/per.458]
Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2006). Relationships between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: A comparative study of five specific samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(8), 1659-1669. [DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2005.12.009]
Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2006) Cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire-development of a short 18- item version (CERQ-short). Personality and Individual Differences, 41(6), 1045-1053. [DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2006.04.010]
Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2007) Cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire, psychometric features and prospective relationships with depression and anxiety in adults. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 141-149. [DOI:10.1027/1015-5759.23.3.141]
Ghasempour, A. (2012). [Predicting death anxiety on the basis of emotion cognitive regulation strategies (Persian)]. Knowledge & Research in Applied Psychology, 13(2), 63-70. [Link]
Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3-24). New York: Guilford. [Link]
Hartley, C. A., & Phelps, E. A. (2010). Changing fear: The neurocircuitry of emotion regulation. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 136-146. [DOI:10.1038/npp.2009.121] [PMID]
Hasani, J. (2010). [Examining the reliability and validity of the short form of the emotion cognitive order questionnaire (pERSIAN)]. Behavioral Science Research, 9(4), 229-240. [Link]
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavior disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(6), 1152–1168. [DOI:10.1037/0022-006X.64.6.1152] [PMID]
Hayes, S. C., Strohsahl, K., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., Pistorello, J., & Toarmino, D., et al. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance. A preliminary test of a working model. Th Psychological Record, 54, 553-578. [Link]
Hayes-Skelton, S. A., Orsillo, S. M., & Roemer, L. (2013). An acceptance based behavioral therapy for individuals with Generalized Anxiety Disorder. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 20(3), 264–281. [DOI:10.1016/j.cbpra.2011.02.005] [PMID]
Heeren, A., Billieux, J., Philippot, P., De Raedt, R., Baeken, C., & de Timary, P., et al. (2017). Impact of transcranial direct current stimulation on attentional bias for threat: A proof-of-concept study among individuals with social anxiety disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(2), 251-260. [DOI:10.1093/scan/nsw119] [PMID]
Hoffart, A., Johnson, S, U., Nordahl, H, M., & Wells, A. (2018). Mechanisms of change in metacognitive and cognitive behavioral therapy for treatment-resistant anxiety: the role of metacognitive beliefs and coping strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 9(3). [DOI:10.1177/2043808718787414]
Ironside, M., Browning, M., Ansari, T. L., Harvey, C. J., Sekyi-Djan, M. N., & Bishop, S. J., et al. (2019). Effect of prefrontal cortex stimulation on regulation of amygdala response to threat in individuals with trait anxiety: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(1), 71-78. [DOI:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.2172] [PMID]
Jacobs, N., Kleen, M., De Groot, F., & A-Tjak, J. (2008). Het meten van experiëntiële vermijding: De Nederlandstalige versie van de Acceptance and Action Ouestionnaire-II (AAO-II) [The measurement of experiential avoidance: The Dutch language version of the Acceptance and Action Ouestionnaire-II (AAQ-II)]. Gedragstherapie, 41(4), 349-361. [Link]
Johnson, S. U., Hoffart, A., Nordahl, H. M., Ulvenes, P. G., Vrabel, K., & Wampold, B. E. (2018). Metacognition and cognition in inpatient MCT and CBT for comorbid anxiety disorders: A study of within-person effects. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 65(1), 86–97. [DOI:10.1037/cou0000226] [PMID]
Jankowski, T., & Holas, P. (2014). Metacognitive model of mindfulness. Consciousness and Cognition, 28, 64-80. [DOI:10.1016/j.concog.2014.06.005] [PMID]
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living, using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Bantam. [Link]
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 144-156. [DOI:10.1093/clipsy.bpg016]
Liebetanz, D., Nitsche, M. A., Tergau, F., & Paulus, W. (2002). Pharmacological approach to the mechanisms of transcranial DC-stimulation-induced after-effects of human motor cortex excitability. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 125(Pt 10), 2238–2247. [DOI:10.1093/brain/awf238] [PMID]
Lotfi, S., Abolghasemi, A., Narimani, M. (2013). [A comparison of emotional processing and fear of positive/negative evaluations in women with social phobia and normal women (Persian)]. Knowledge & Research in Applied Psychology, 14(3), 101-111. [Link]
Marques, L. M., Morello, L. Y. N., & Boggio, P. S. (2018). Ventrolateral but not Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex tDCS effectively impact emotion reappraisal - effects on Emotional Experience and Inter beat Interval. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 15295. [DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-33711-5] [PMID]
Martin, D. M., Liu, R., Alonzo, A., Green, M., Player, M. J., & Sachdev, P., et al. (2013). Can transcranial direct current stimulation enhance outcomes from cognitive training? A randomized controlled trial in healthy participants. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 16(9), 1927–1936.[DOI:10.1017/S1461145713000539] [PMID]
Mash, E., & Wolfe, D. (2012). Abnormal child psychology. Massachusetts: Cengage Learning. [Link]
McCluskey, D. L., Haliwa, I., Wilson, J. Keeley, J. W., & Shook, N. J. (2022). Experiential avoidance mediates the relation between mindfulness and anxiety. Current Psychology, 41, 3947-3957. [Link]
Mennin, D. S. (2004). Emotion regulation therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 11(1), 17-29. [DOI:10.1002/cpp.389]
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167-202. [DOI:10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167] [PMID]
Naeinian, M. R., Shaeiri, M. R., Sharif, M., & Hadian, M. (2011). [To study reliability and validity for a brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder (GAD-7) (Persian)]. Clinical Psychology & Personality, 9(1), 41-50. [Link]
Nazaribadie, M., Ghaliha, A., Ahmadpanah, M., Mazdeh, M., Matinnia, N., & Zarabian, M. K. (2020). Effectiveness of detached mindfulness intervention on cognitive functions in multiple sclerosis patients, results from a randomized controlled study. Pakistan Journal of Medical and Health Sciences, 14(4), 78-94. [Link]
Normann, N., van Emmerik, A. A., & Morina, N. (2014). The efficacy of metacognitive therapy for anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. Depression and Anxiety, 31(5), 402–411.[DOI:10.1002/da.22273] [PMID]
Nordahl, H. M., Borkovec, T. D., Hagen, R., Kennair, L. E. O., Hjemdal, O., & Solem, S., et al. (2018). Metacognitive therapy versus cognitive-behavioral therapy in adults with generalized anxiety disorder. BJPsych Open, 4(5), 393–400.[DOI:10.1192/bjo.2018.54] [PMID]
Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 242–249.[DOI:10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010] [PMID]
Ochsner, K. N., Hughes, B., Robertson, E. R., Cooper, J. C., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2009). Nutral systems supporting the control of affective and cognitive conflicts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(9), 1842–1855. [DOI:10.1162/jocn.2009.21129] [PMID]
Ollendick, T. H., & Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R. (2002). The developmental psychopathology of social anxiety disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 44-58. [DOI:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01305-1.] [PMID]
Orsillo, S. M., & Roemer, L. (2005). Acceptance and mindfulness-based approaches to anxiety. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. [DOI:10.1007/b136521]
Osinsky, R., Gebhardt, H., Alexander, N., & Hennig, J. (2012). Trait anxiety and the dynamics of attentional control. Biological Psychology, 89(1), 252–259. [DOI:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.016] [PMID]
Peña-Gómez, C., Vidal-Piñeiro, D., Clemente, I. C., Pascual-Leone, Á., & Bartrés-Faz, D. (2011). Down-regulation of negative emotional processing by transcranial direct current stimulation: Effects of personality characteristics. PLoS One, 6(7), e22812. [DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0022812] [PMID]
Polanía, R., Paulus, W., & Nitsche, M. A. (2012). Modulating cortico-striatal and thalamo-cortical functional connectivity with transcranial direct current stimulation. Human Brain Mapping, 33(10), 2499-2508. [DOI:10.1002/hbm.21380] [PMID]
Rabie, M., Zerehposh, A., Palahang, H., & Zarei, H. (2013). [Relationship between components of cognitive emotion regulation and anxiety disorders in child and adolescent (Persian)]. Journal of Research in Behavioral Sciences, 11(5), 363-74. [Link]
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: A new approach to preventing relapse. New York: The Guilford Press. [Link]
Sotres-Bayon, F., Quirk, G. J. (2010). Prefrontal control of fear: more than just extinction. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(2), 231–235. [DOI:10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.005] [PMID]
Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B., & Löwe, B. (2006). A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Archives of Internal Medicine, 166(10), 1092–1097.[DOI:10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092] [PMID]
Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., & Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: Empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(2), 275-287. [DOI:10.1037/0022-006X.70.2.275] [PMID]
Urry, H. L., van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., Kalin, N. H., Thurow, M. E., & Schaefer, H. S., et al. (2006). Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 26(16), 4415–4425. [DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3215-05.2006] [PMID]
Vicario, C. M., Salehinejad, M. A., Felmingham, K., Martino, G., & Nitsche, M. A. (2019). A systematic review on the therapeutic effectiveness of non-invasive brain stimulation for the treatment of anxiety disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 96, 219-231. [DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.012] [PMID]
Watkins, E., Teasdale, J. D., & Williams, R. M. (2000). Decentring and distraction reduce overgeneral autobiographical in depression. Psychological Medicine, 30(4), 911-920. [DOI:10.1017/s0033291799002263] [PMID]
Wells A. (2009). Metacognitive therapy for anxiety and depression. Guilford Press. [Link]
Weber, M. J., Messing, S. B., Rao, H., Detre, J. A., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014). Prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation alters activation and connectivity in cortical and subcortical reward systems: A tDCS-fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 35(8), 3673-3686. [DOI:10.1002/hbm.22429] [PMID]
Type of Study: Research | Subject: Psychiatry
Received: 2023/10/8 | Accepted: 2023/12/5 | Published: 2024/04/1

Add your comments about this article : Your username or Email:
CAPTCHA

Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb