Volume 8, Issue 1 (Winter 2020)                   PCP 2020, 8(1): 9-16 | Back to browse issues page

XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Rajabi G, Naderi Nobandegani Z. Reliability and Validity of the Persian Version of Reasons for Death Fear Scale. PCP 2020; 8 (1) :9-16
URL: http://jpcp.uswr.ac.ir/article-1-568-en.html
1- Department of Counseling, Faculty of Education & Psychology, Shahid Chamran University, Ahvaz, Iran. , rajabireza@scu.ac.ir
2- Department of Psychology, Yazd University, Yazd, Iran.
Full-Text [PDF 552 kb]   (1481 Downloads)     |   Abstract (HTML)  (2363 Views)
Full-Text:   (859 Views)
1. Introduction
Fear of death is a global phenomenon, and the main subject in thanatology, in every part of the world you would find many people who are afraid of dying. There is a clear reason for the fear of death: Death is the strangest phenomenon in the world, surrounded by numerous unknown variables, and it is the inevitable part of the human life (Dezutter, Luyckx, & Hutsebaut, 2009). Anthropologist Ernest Becker (1973) argued that “the idea of death, the fear of it, haunts the human animal like nothing else. Humans are social beings, and it is our interactions with other humans that complete our existence and give our lives meaning’’ (Moore & Williamson, 2003). Margaret Paul (2012) describes the fear of death as stemming from two beliefs: ‘When we die, we disappear’ and ‘when we die we will be punished for our sins’. Kastenbaum and Aisenberg (1972) identified six specific death-related fears from a self-other orientation that include a fear of dying, a fear about what might happen following death, and a fear of extinction. Choron (1974) distinguished three varieties of death fear: (1) what happens after death, (2) fear of dying, and (3) fear of ceasing to be. Hoelter and Hoelter (1978) distinguish eight dimensions of the death fear: fear of the dying process, fear of premature death, fear for significant others, phobic fear of death, fear of being destroyed, fear of the body after death, fear of the unknown, and fear of the dead. Similarly, Florian and Mikulincer (1993) identified three components of the death fear: intrapersonal components related to the impact of death on the mind and the body, which include fears of loss of fulfillment of personal goals and fear of the body’s annihilation; an interpersonal component that is related to the effect of death on interpersonal relationships; and a transpersonal component that concerns with fears about the transcendental self, composed of fears about the hereafter and punishment after death. 
Wong (2008) presented different aspects regarding the meaning of death fears; these aspects are rooted death anxiety. The 10 meanings he proposed were finality, uncertainty, amihilitation, ultimate lose, life flow loneliness, and judgment and retribution. Compbellm (2013) stated the following reasons for death fear: the unknown loneliness and anxiety from tolerating the death experience; loss of family and friends; the loss of self-control of bodily functions; possibility of suffering; and the failure to achieve one's life goals.
Also, various studies consider variables that are related to death such as death anxiety (Abdl-khalek & Tomas-Sabado, 2005; Chen, Benk, & Lewis, 2006; Rajabi & Bahrani, 2002 Templer, 1970; death obsession (Abdel-khalek, 1998; Abdel-khalek, 2002b; Abdel-khalek, Al-Arja, & Abdalla, 2006; Maltby & Day, 2000; Rajabi, 2009), death depression (Almostadi, 2012; Alvarado, Templer, Bresler, & Thomas-Dobson, 1995; Rajabi, Begdeli, & Naderi Nobandegani, 2015; Rajabi & Naderi Nobandegani, in press), death distress (Abdl-khalek, 2004; Abdl-khalek, 2005; Al-Sabwah & Abdel-khalek, 2005-2006), attitude toward death (Abu Hasheesh, Al-Sayed AboZeid, Goda El-Zaid, & Alhujaili, 2013; Dezutter et al, 2009; Neimeyer, Wittkowski, & Moser, 2004). 
Since the late 1950s, many studies have been undertaken to determine if religiosity is associated with fear of death (Ellis & Wahab, 2013). Meanwhile, because many psychological theories have considered human concern about death and dying as related with religion, it seems that there is a specific relationship between individuals’ attitude toward the religion and their view on the death problem (Dezutter et al., 2009). Moreover, according to the results of the studies, there may be a relationship between death fear-related variables such as death anxiety and depression and the religion (Maltby & Day, 2002). The results of Power and Smith's research (2008) revealed that the more religious participants had higher fear of the dead, fear of being destroyed, and fear of conscious death, whereas participants with lower religious conviction were more fearful of the unknown. 
First time, Abdel-khalek (2002a) developed a scale to investigate the Reasons for Death Fear among Muslims. The sample consisted of 307 university undergraduate students recruited from Egypt, Kuwait, Lebanon, and Saudi Arabia. After investigating the responses of these participants, the items that were ambiguous, repeated and irrelevant were omitted and rests of the items were edited. Afterwards, the preliminary version of the scale was forwarded to four staff members in the Psychology Department of Kuwait University to judge its suitability. Then, the scale was administered to 255 male and female undergraduate students. In this stage, a number of items were omitted again. The final form of the scale consisted of 18 items. Abdel-Khalek identified four factors: fear of pain and punishment, losing worldly involvement, consequences of religious, and failures, being parted from loved ones that are among major factors of death fear for Arabic or Muslim college students.
Although, in Iran, various studies (Rajabi et al., 2015; Rajabi & Bahrani, 2002; Roshdieh, Templer, & Cannon, 1999) have been done on Iranian sample about death, but no study directly considered the reasons of death in Muslims society of Iran. In this study, therefore, our target is study of reliability and validity of the reasons for fear death in Iranian samples. This study expands upon this previous research and aims to answer the following questions: (1) what is the optimal factor structure for the Reasons for Death Fear Scale, as determined by exploratory factor analysis? (2) Does the Persian version of the Reasons for Death Fear Scale have an appropriate convergent, divergent validity and reliability?
2. Methods
The present study was psychometric of methodological type.The sample of this study using were selected a convenience sample of 157 participants (53 males’ 33.8% and 104 females 66.2%) among 310 the participants that attending in religious rituals of Mosques in Shiraz, Iran and by Include criteria willingness to participate in the study, attend religious rituals at least three times in a month, not having physical illness, having no emotional and family problems. The mean age of participants was 31.54 (SD = 11.09), with the range of 18 to 60 years; for men, it was 29.23 (SD = 10.36) with the range of 18 to 55 years and for women 36.08 (SD = 11.09) with the range of 20 to 60 years.
The Reasons for Death Fear Scale (RDFS). This scale was developed by Abdel-khalek in 2002 and consists of 18 items. These items have been scaled on a 5-point Likert scale (from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). In a factor analysis, Abdel-khalek (2002) identified 4 reasons for death fear: fear of pain and punishment, fear of losing worldly involvements, religious transgressions and failures, and parting from loved ones. He obtained 0.80 for Cronbach's alpha reliability coefficients of the scale. The validity coefficients of RDFS with the Death Anxiety Scale by Templer (1970), the Death Depression Scale-Revised by Templer, the Death Obsession Scale (Abdel-Khalek, 1998b; Maltby & Day, 2000), the Arabic Scale of Death Anxiety (Abdel-Khalek, 1996), and the Kuwait University Anxiety Scale (Abdel-Khalek, 2000) were statistically significant. The items of the Farsi Reasons for Death Fear Scale were separately translated into Farsi by the authors. The translated versions were then compared to the original text by a language expert, fluent in English, and the words were modified and confirmed as adequately representing the original items. The primary Farsi version was again translated from Farsi into English and compared to the original form, which showed high similarity between the two forms. 
Trend of research
Before submitting questionnaire to participants about the purpose of research and its application in this particular group, the information was provided individually. The participations was also mentioned that they could participate freely and with complete satisfaction. They also provided information on how to respond to each scale, and they were informed that their personal information and scales would be confidential and that the data would be analyzed collectively. To observe ethical standards, after obtaining informed consent from the participants, the questionnaires were completed without lastname to ensure the confidentiality of the information.
 In this study, the data were analyzed using SPSS-23 and applied to the study of psychometric characteristics of the Reasons for Death Fear Scale: (1) A Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was ran to make ensure of normality of distribution of the data. (2) Exploratory factor analysis with Varimax rotation was performed to identify underlying factors and preliminarily established factorial validity. (3) Cronbach΄s alphas were calculated to test internal consistency of the measure. (4) Finally, convergent and divergent validity of the scale were assessed by examining correlations between the Reasons for Death Fear Scale with extracted factors and the factors with each other.
3. Results 
The mean and standard deviation total score on the RDES was 61.57 and 11.50.  The lowest mean item score was 2.37 for item 16 ("Grieving over what one will leave behind, e.g. wealth, valuables, etc."), and the highest mean item score was 4.11 for item 7 ("Fear of hell and doomsday"). A Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to ensure that the collected data has a normal distribution. The results (z = 0.94, p< 0.339) showed that the distribution of the collected data is not significantly different from normal. The KMO index (0.83) and Bartlett test of Sphericity (  p= 1029.50, p< 0.001), showed that the assumed normal distribution has been observed, and the sample size is sufficient for factor analysis. Table 1 shows the factors for the Reasons for Death Fear Scale.
As shown in Table 1, the four extracted factors explain 59.43% of the variance of RDFS items. The first factor explained 31.60% of the variance, and based on item content, it was labeled Fear of Pain and Punishment. Items of the second factor accounted for 12.45% variance, and this factor was termed Fear of Losing Worldly Pleasure and Investments. The third factor accounted for 8.12% of the variance and was labeled Fear of not doing Religious Virtues. And, the fourth factor accounted for 6.25% of the variance and was labeled Fear of Parting from Loved Ones. Thus, the RDFS was found to be a multi-dimensional scale. It should be noted that 2 items (items 5 and 12) have loads on the second factor, but based on items content, item 5 can be placed on the fourth factor, and item 12 has load on the second factor, but it can be loaded on the third factor based on item content in terms of factor loadings higher than 0.40 (Table 1). 

The alpha coefficients were 0.86 for the total scale, 0.81 for Fear of Pain and Punishment factor (5 items), 0.72 for Fear of Losing Worldly Pleasure and Investments factor (4 items), 0.77 for Fear of not doing Religious Virtues (5 items), and 0.70 for Fear of Parting from Loved Ones. Overall, these results indicate acceptable levels of internal consistency (Table 1).


4. Discussion
The scale of RDFS has a good internal consistency and the Cronbach's α coefficient calculated from the sample confirm this finding about of whole scale and the subscales. I doing so, constant factors were extracted from the RDFS and named as fear of pain and punishment, fear of loss of investment and earthly delights, fear of not doing of religious obligations, and fear of being separated from loved ones. Less and more, these factors have cognitive, social and religious aspects. The four extracted factors and their meanings are likely considered as indices of adequacy of items, internal consistency of RDFS and good factorial structure of the scale in measuring one consistent concept (reasons for death fear). Prior studies on the RDFS among Egyptian, Kuwaiti, and Iranian university students also reported good reliability (Abdel-Khalek, 2002A; Aflakseir, 2014).
The principal component analysis showed 4-factor "Fear of Pain and Punishment," "Fear of Losing Worldly Pleasure and Investments," "Fear of not doing Religious Virtues," and "Fear of Parting from Loved Ones". These identified components were consistent with the theoretical formulation of the scale and the four components of Abdel-Khalek (2002), and Aflaksair (2014).
One of the basic principles of Islam is to believe in afterlife. In this far live and in the resurrection day the soul and body would be revived, and after a final trade-off, reward or punish (paradise or hell) would be become every man eternal destiny. Afterlife is immortal and infinite live of man. This life is very certain and after being buried in the ground would began. This ground, depend on actions done in the world, would end up somewhere near the heaven or somewhere in deep valleys of hell. In the latter one, grave suffering, i.e. torture of grave would occur. In the Islam religion, Muslims believe that the God would create life and death. They also believe that death is visiting God and is a transition from earth mortal life to the eternal life in heaven or hell. Every true Muslim believes that God has determined the exact time of death for everyone. Nobody, however, knows the time (Al-Sabwah & Abdel-Khalek, 2006).
With the exception of Abdel-Khalek (2002A), and Aflaksier (2014) works there is no empirical evidence directly assigned to the research question 'why we fear of death?' Thus, this study was succeeding in accomplishing the research main goal (construct validity).
There are significant positive correlations of RDFS to the other scales that indicate its convergent validity. These results are consistent with the results of Abdel-Khalek (2002A), Abdel-Khalek (2004), Aflakseir (2014), and Ayyad (2013) who found RDFS to be significantly and positively correlated to the Death Anxiety Scale, Death Depression Scale, Death Distress Scale and Death Obsession Scale. 
Based on voluntarily selection of people participated in religious rituals in Shiraz, Iran, 18 items of the Reasons for Death Fear Scale (RDFS) were analyzed after the initial translation and several times modification of the items for their contents (exploratory factor analysis). Therefore, we certainly concluded that the RDFS appropriately represents the participants' views in response to the question, 'why we fear of death?' Thus, the generalizability of the current finding to other samples similar to Iranians, show in a high extent that what are the possible similarities between the current findings and other participants from other cultures, places, languages, and religious orientations for reasons of linguistic commonalities and being Muslim. In other words, their common major meaning in the country is Persian language and they are Muslims just like the other adjacent countries, especially the Arab ones.
All of religions have paid attention to the issue fear of death across the world; for instance, in a part of Christians' teachings it has come: everybody in the world will face death in the last moments of their lives, and the moment is terrifying and fearful (Brantl, 1961). Islam is not an exception. In this religion, the issues death and fear of it are viewed intrinsic and, of course, considerable; thus, in Muslims' holy book and elderlies' speeches of this religion, the issue is preceded a lot. According to various verses of Qur'an about death being unavoidable, pains related to death moment and dying, auditing to sins in another world, and time and place of death being unknown, it is inferred that unawareness and unrecognition of nature of death, attachment to the world, fear of death pains, perpetrated sins, and fear of the auditing are some main reasons of fear of death.
Beg and Zilli (1982) have stated that "the pains and agonies of the grave are many and unknown" (p. 122). In the religious Islam, it is true that these pains and agonies are many, but they are not unknown, inasmuch as many saying Ahadeath of the prophet of Islam elucidate the different forms of Azab Al-Qabr. Thus, it is predictable that item 2 (i.e. "Worry about one's offspring"), item 1 (i.e. "fear of heavenly punishment") and Items 14 and 15 (i.e. "Torture of the grave", " Acute pains associated with dying") had primary, second and third highest ranks among the items of the RDFS, respectively.
5. Conclusion
There are some limitations in this research. First, it is important to note that the research has been done on women and men attending in religious rituals (e.g. mosques and prays) and the generalization of the results to other populations should be done cautionary. Lack of test-retest reliability coefficient of RDFS is another limitation making hard the generalization to other samples with age, educational, occupational, and ethnic characteristics. The current findings are particularly related to Iranian Muslim papulation, thus, we recommend studying the scale in other cultures (western and nonwestern ones). It is suggested that researcher's evaluate this scale in different age groups and populations. Another suggestion is to use the retest reliability method to investigation this scale over time.
Ethical Considerations
Compliance with ethical guidelines
All ethical principles were considered in this article. The participants were informed about the purpose of the research and its implementation stages; they were also assured about the confidentiality of their information; Moreover, They were allowed to leave the study whenever they wish, and if desired, the results of the research would be available to them.
Funding
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Authors' contributions
All authors contributed equally in preparing all parts of the research.
Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.


References
Bailes, S., Creti, L., Fichten, C., S., Libman, E., Brender, W., & Amsel, R. (2010). Sexual self-efficacy scale for female functioning. In T. D. Fisher, C., M., Davis, W. L. Yarber, S., L., Davis (Eds.). Handbook of sexuality-related measures. New York: Routledge.
Basson, R. (2001). Female sexual response: The role of drugs in the management of sexual dysfunction. Obstetrics & Gynecology, 98(2), 350-53. [DOI:10.1097/00006250-200108000-00029]
Creti, L., Bailes, S., Fichten, C., Libman, E., Amsel, R., Liederman., & Brender, W. (1989). Validation of the sexual self-efficacy scale for females. Poster presented at: The annual convention of the American Psychological Association, New Orleans, LA.
Creti, L., Fichten, C., Libman, E., Kalogeropoulos, D., & Brender, W. (1987). A global score for the “sexual History Form” and its effectiveness. Presented at: The 21st annual convention of the association for advancement of behavior therapy. Boston, Mass.
Fichten, C., S., Budd, J., Spector, I., Amsel, R., Creti, L., Brender W., et al. (2010). Sexual self-efficacy scale-erectile functioning. In: T. D Fisher, C. M., Davis, W. L., Yarber, L., Davis (Eds.). Handbook of Sexuality-related Measures. New York: Routledge.
Fichten, C., S., Libman, E., & Rothenberg, I. (1988). Sexual self-efficacy scale-erectile functioning. In: C. M. Davis, W. I., Yarber, & S. I. Davis (Eds.), Sexuality Related Measures: A compendium (pp. 129-131). 
Kimmel, D., & Van der Veen, F. (1974). Factor of marital adjustment. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 36, 57-63.
Libman, E., Rothenberg, I., Fichten, C., S., & Amsel, R. (1985). The SSESE: A measure of sexual self-efficacy in erectile functioning. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 11, 233-247. [DOI:10.1080/00926238508405450] [PMID]
LoPiccolo, J., & Steger, J., C. (1974). The sexual interaction inventory: A new instrument for assessment of sexual dysfunction. Archives of Sex Behavior, 3, 585-95. [DOI:10.1007/BF01541141] [PMID]
Nowinski, J., K., & LoPiccolo, J. (1979). Assessing sexual behaviors in couples. Journal of Sex Marital Therapy, 5, 225-43. [DOI:10.1080/00926237908403731] [PMID]
Phillips, N., A. (2000). Female sexual dysfunction: Evaluation and treatment. American Family Physician, l62, 127-36.
Quirk, F., H., Heiman, J., R., Rosen, R., C., Laan, E., Smith, M., D., & Boolell, M. (2002). Development of a sexual function questionnaire for clinical trials of female sexual dysfunction. Journal of Women’s Health & Gender-Based Medicine, 41, 277-91. [DOI:10.1089/152460902753668475] [PMID]
Rosen, R., Brown, C., Heiman, J., Leiblum, S., Meston, C., Shabsigh, R., et al. (2000). The Female Sexual Function Index (FSFI): A multidimensional self-report instrument for the assessment of female sexual function. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 26, 191-208. [DOI:10.1080/009262300278597] [PMID]
Rosenthal, D., Moore, S., & Flynn, I. (1991). Adolescent self-efficacy, self-esteem and sexual risk-taking. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 1, 77-8. [DOI:10.1002/casp.2450010203]
Rust, J., & Golombok, S. (1985). The Golombok Rust Inventory of Sexual Satisfaction (GRISS). British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 24, 63-4. [DOI:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1985.tb01314.x] [PMID]
Taylor, J., F., Rosen, R., C., & Leiblum, S. R. (1994). Self-report assessment of female sexual function: Psychometric evaluation of the Brief Index of Sexual Functioning for Women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23, 627-43. [DOI:10.1007/BF01541816] [PMID]
Vermillion, S., T., S., & Holmes, M., M. (1997). Sexual dysfunction in women. Primary Care Update for OB/GYNS, 4, 234-40. [DOI:10.1016/S1068-607X(97)00104-2]
Type of Study: Research | Subject: Psychometric
Received: 2019/06/23 | Accepted: 2019/10/23 | Published: 2020/01/1

Add your comments about this article : Your username or Email:
CAPTCHA

Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb