Volume 10, Issue 3 (Summer 2022)                   PCP 2022, 10(3): 185-192 | Back to browse issues page


XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Abdolmohammadi K, Malek A, Ghadiri Sourman Abadi F, Ebrahimzadeh K. Comparing the Dimensions of Executive Functions in Monolingual and Bilingual Children. PCP 2022; 10 (3) :185-192
URL: http://jpcp.uswr.ac.ir/article-1-813-en.html
1- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Educational Sciences and Psychology, Azarbaijan Shahid Madani University, Tabriz, Iran.
2- Research Center of Psychiatry and Bahavioral Sciences, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, Iran.
3- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Literature and Humanities, Urmia University, Urmia, Iran. , farhadghadiri17@yahoo.com
4- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Literature and Humanities, Urmia University, Urmia, Iran.
Full-Text [PDF 566 kb]   (700 Downloads)     |   Abstract (HTML)  (1813 Views)
Full-Text:   (460 Views)
1. Introduction
The interest in comprehending the distinctions among executive functions has been increasing gradually. Executive function is a generalized term that includes distinct but closely related components, when considered together, form a common pattern of functionality (Friedman, Mike, 2017). The literature has considered the core components of executive functions as working memory, cognitive inhibition and flexibility, attentional shift, emotional control, initiation, strategic planning, organization of materials, and monitoring (Devine, Ribner, Hughes, 2019). These multidimensional cognitive structures are essential for purposeful behavior and problem-solving in all dimensions of life, whether academic, professional, or social (Fogel, et al., 2020).
Because a close relationship exists between the development of executive functions and the prefrontal cortex, it has long been believed that executive functions develop during adolescence (Bernier, Carlson, & Whipple, 2010). However, with the growth of Cognitive Neuroscience, it has gradually become clear that the development of executive functions begins much earlier, well before primary school and even as early as 3 years of age (Fiske, Holmboe, 2019). There is even evidence that executive functional development may start to form from infancy (Diamond, 2013) up to adolescence (Carlson, Zelazo, Faja, 2013). 
Also, the environment influences childhood development at different periods. Environmental factors may have a greater impact on the development of executive functions during early childhood (Müller, Baker, Yeung, 2013). Regarding the role and impact of executive functions on people’s behavioral inhibition and personal growth, it is important to understand the factors influencing the development of executive functions (Halse, 2014). The general consensus suggested that executive functions developed in interacting with the environment (Bernier et al., 2010). Recently scientific and impartial research has shown that environmental conditions have a significant impact on the development of executive functions (Blair, Raver, 2012). Studies have also shown that factors such as parenting (Sosic-Vasic, et al., 2017), socioeconomic status (Last, et al., 2018), motor development (Abdolmohammadi, et al., 2020) and preterm birth (Abdolmohammadi, et al., 2019) affect the development of executive functions.
Of the many environmental factors that influence the development of the executive function, bilingualism is of particular interest to researchers (Antón, Carreiras, Duñabeitia, 2019). Bilingualism, or multilingualism, is the ability to use two or more languages ​​to communicate with others alternatively (depending on specific situations). This social lingual phenomenon is caused by social behaviors that affect how a person speaks (Ortega, 2019). Bilingualism is a phenomenon that exists in all countries and engages about half of the world’s population (Delphi, Delphi, Zarifian, Bakhshi, 2018). Various studies show that bilingualism has many beneficial cognitive effects on executive functions (Anton et al., 2019), metacognitive awareness (Antoniou, 2019), divergent thinking (Lee, Kim, 2011), phonetic perception (Antoniou et al., 2015), control of cognitive attention, and flexibility (Adi-Japha, Berberich, Libnawi, 2010).
The results of the previous studies on the relationship between executive functions and bilingualism have been inconsistent. For example, Bialystok (2017) and Kubota, Chevalier, and Sorace (2020) found that bilingual children show better performance with respect to executive functions, while Arizmendi, et al., (2018) Nichols, et al., (2020) found no difference in terms of executive functions among bilinguals and monolinguals. This discrepancy between results suggests that the relationship between bilingualism and the development of executive functions requires further research (DeLuca, et al., 2020).
Objections to the methodologies used within the literature have been raised. For instance, parental literacy and economic status are (often or always?) not controlled within these studies (Daller, Ongun, 2018). Another concern is that the use of small sample sizes makes generalizing the results difficult (Paap, Johnson, Sawi, 2015). Additionally, the tools that are often used within these studies can have low validity and reliability and do not always measure people’s real-life executive functions accurately (Abdolmohamadi, et al., 2018).
Consequently, researchers have shown a positive effect of bilingualism in many studies. Although some people speak a language other than Persian in Iran, little research has been done in this field, and many questions about bilingualism remain unanswered. The present study is important because it draws more attention to proper language teaching. Because bilingualism affects executive functions, the responsibilities of parents and the educational system in terms of language learning are doubled. Therefore, the results of this study have a theoretical value in providing useful information about the level of executive functions in monolingual and bilingual people for the country’s educational system. Practically, it can draw the attention of education officials to the importance, the role of these processes, and the use of appropriate methods in learning. According to the well-documented relationship between executive functions and bilingualism, this study aimed to compare the executive functions between bilingual and monolingual children despite the inconsistency of reported results and potential methodological shortcomings found in the literature.

2. Participants and Methods
This study used a causal-comparative design. Participants were selected by using available sampling. In this study, bilingual children are those who can speak two languages completely. At first, the mothers of bilingual and monolingual children were invited to the school, and after explaining the objectives of the test, they were asked to complete the research questionnaire if they were satisfied. To observe the ethical of research subject rights, the researcher first introduced himself to the students and stated the aim of the research, then both orally (before the performance) and written (mentioned in the first part of the questionnaire) ensured that “The information requested in these questionnaires is for research purposes only. To reassure you, apart from determining your gender and age, you do not need to mention your first and last name and other personal details”.

Research participants
The official language of Iranian society is Persian. However, there are also several unofficial languages with native speakers; they are required to learn the official language as early as less than 5 years old. Therefore, Iranian society is ideal for investigating the cognitive differences between monolingual and bilingual children. Because the test is causal-comparative, Cohen’s table (2013) was used to select the sample size. Although 80 people were enough for each group, according to Cohen’s table, 100 people were selected for each group. A group of 100 children who have Azeri-speaking parents and live in Azeri-speaking areas first mastered the Azeri language and then, being exposed to the Persian language in formal education from the age of 5 was compared with a group of 100 children whose mother tongue was Persian and in harmony with the official and educational language of the country. Both groups were selected from public school students. The education level of mothers of bilingual children comprised diploma 35%, postgraduate 8%, bachelor 48%, master 7%, and doctorate 2%, and of the monolingual children as diploma 38%, postgraduate 5%, bachelor 47%, master 9%, and doctorate 1%. The Mean±SD age of mothers in the bilingual and monolingual groups were 29.78±3.24 and 33.83±2.64 years, respectively. Mothers completed questionnaires due to their more accurate knowledge of children’s characteristics. The inclusion criteria were being bilingual in the bilingual group, being monolingual in the monolingual group, bilingual mothers of bilingual groups, children over 6 and under 12, not being diagnosed with severe mental health problems based on students’ health records, and parents’ literacy level (at least diploma). The exclusion criteria were parents’ dissatisfaction and lack of cooperation. Participants’ demographic information is reported in Table 1.

Study measures
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) 

The questionnaire, developed by Gioia, Isquith, Guy, and Kenworthy (2000), has the parents and teachers’ booklet and assessed performance deficiencies in eight areas: inhibition, attentional shift, emotional control, initiation, working memory, strategic planning, organization of materials, and monitoring. This questionnaire is a tool with high reliability and validity in measuring executive functions, and it has greater value than similar executive function questionnaires due to its assessment of people’s real-life behavior. This questionnaire is scored on a modified 3-point Likert scale (never=0, sometimes=1, and often=2) (Memisevic & Sinanovic, 2013).
The questionnaire responses are subdivided into two categorical indicators: Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) (consisting of inhibition, attentional shift, and emotional control) and Cognitive Index (CI) (consisting of initiation, working memory, strategic planning, organization of materials, and monitoring). Finally, a score of 8 on the scale and the two indices are used to obtain the total score for the BRIEF test (Memisevic, 2015).
The mean Cronbach alpha was between 0.82 and 0.98, and the correlation coefficient obtained after retesting 3 weeks from the initial assessment for parent’s scales was between 0.72 and 0.84. This retesting result supports this questionnaire’s superior validity and reliability compared to previous executive function questionnaires (Gioia et al., 2000). This questionnaire was standardized by Abdolmohammadi et al. (2019) in Iran, with the results showing that the Cronbach alpha, calculated for BRIEF questionnaire scales, is between 0.68 and 0.86. These results indicate that the internal consistency of the questionnaire’s scales falls within the appropriate range. The present questionnaire used in this study obtains two indices and a total score in addition to separately measured scales of executive functions. For this questionnaire, the Cronbach alpha calculated for the BRI, the CI, and the total score of the BRIEF questionnaire were 0.86, 0.89, and 0.93, respectively, indicating good internal consistency.

3. Results
First, descriptive statistics of the research variables are presented in Table 1. Wilkes’ Lambda test was used to assess the differences between the two groups in Table 2, and Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) was applied to compare the components of executive functions between the two groups participating in Table 3
Table 1 shows that the monolingual group scored higher than the bilingual children in total score and all executive function components though there was a slight difference in socioeconomic status between bilingual and monolingual children on test. It showed that this difference was not statistically significant (t=1.38, df=198, P<0.16).
Multivariate analysis of variance was used to compare differences in executive functions between subject groups (monolingual and bilingual). We used Wilks’ Lambda to determine F in MANOVA. Pillai’s trace, Wilks’ lambda, Hotelling’s trace, and Roy’s largest root were used to investigate the significant difference between the groups participating in the study.
In Table 2, the Wilkes’ lambda results (P<0.0001, F8,105=10.17) show that the hypothesis regarding the similarity of society’s means based on dependent variables for the two groups is rejected. Then the results of the intergroup variance analysis test were used to determine which of the variables was significantly different between the two groups.

As Table 3 shows, higher scores were achieved for all executive function components and total scores of monolingual children compared to bilingual children (P<0.001). These results strongly imply that bilingual children showed better executive functional performance across all factors when compared to monolingual children.

4. Discussion
This study aimed to compare the executive functions of bilingual and monolingual children. The results showed that bilingual children performed better than monolingual children in all components of executive functions, and they achieved a higher total score. These findings are consistent with the findings of multiple studies, including Salvatira, Rosselli, 2011; Woumans, Duyck, 2015; Krizman, Skoe, Kraus, 2016; Chung-Fat, Sorge and Bialystok, 2017; Crespo, et al., 2019; Kubota et al., 2020. However, there are still a few studies that show conflicting results (Arizmandi et al., 2018; Nichols et al., 2020). The findings of this study further support Astana’s theory claiming that bilingualism is a significant factor in superior cognitive performance (Baker, 2006). 
In Iran, students are required to use a second language academically while primarily using their mother tongues outside school, either based on interest or social pressure leading bilingual children to develop fluency in both languages. This mastery of both languages resulted in increased cognitive development in bilingual children. Furthermore, research has shown that extra exposure to two languages during childhood positively impacts executive functional development (Crespo et al., 2019). One study supporting this claim conducted on Serbian students showed that children who were exposed to a second language for 5 hours a day showed better executive functions than children who were exposed to a second language for one and a half hours (Purić, Vuksanović, Chondrogianni, 2017). In Iran, because of school attendance and mass media, bilingual children are exposed to a second language for more than 5 hours a day. Based on this fact, the results of this study should not be surprising.
Participants in the present study all started to learn a second language before age five. Previous research has shown that bilingual people who learn a second language before the age of 9 have better performance of executive functions. However, children who learn a second language after the age of nine show functional performance closer to that of monolingual children (Salvatira & Rosselli, 2011).
Various factors could underlie the superior performance of bilingual children in terms of executive functions. Among bilingual children, an important factor is the cognitive use of both languages, which is constantly active during language processing, leading to more consistent language monitoring among bilingual children (Kubota et al., 2020). Additional research also shows that bilingual individuals must continually attend to one language over the other while actively inhibiting usage of the unattended language (Woumans & Duyck, 2015). The situational circumstance must be considered when choosing which language to be used. This selective attention is another important factor considering bilingualism which affects executive functions (Chung-Fat et al., 2017). There is also a high displacement between the two languages, both in terms of vocabulary and grammatical structure (Bialystok, 2017). Some researchers have suggested that displacement between the two languages leads to greater cognitive flexibility among bilingual speakers (Rosselli, et al., 2015). Executive functions manage all of the bilingual factors mentioned here; it is this higher reliance on executive functions during language processing that likely results in the overall strengthening of executive function performance.
Neurological studies also support this by showing greater cortical thickness of the prefrontal cortex in bilingual speakers than in monolingual speakers. According to the role of the prefrontal cortex in executive processing, the increase in gray matter volume provides some neurological explanation regarding the relationship between executive functions and bilingualism (Olulade, et al., 2016).

5. Conclusion
The results showed that bilingual and monolingual children differ in terms of executive functions. Bilingual children showed better performance on the skills related to executive functions compared to monolingual children. Considering the superiority of bilingual children in executive functions over monolingual children, it is necessary to plan early teaching of a second language in preschool education, and appropriate interventions should be designed and implemented according to the developmental characteristics of this age group.
The present study also has some limitations. First, it is the cross-sectional nature of the research method. Longitudinal research is vital because previous longitudinal studies have had different results than cross-sectional studies. Second, the sample group generalizes the results of the present study only to the 6 to 12 years old children. Further research needs to use other age groups and different samples. The third limitation is the collection of information from parents. It is better to use methods for collecting information from students in future research. The fourth is using questionnaires; using other tools in future research is recommended. 

Ethical Considerations
Compliance with ethical guidelines

The present study was a comparative causal type study approved by the Regional Committee of Medical Ethics of Tabriz University of Medical Sciences (Code: IR.TBZMED.REC.1398.311.

Funding
This research did not receive any grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or non-profit sectors.

Authors' contributions
All authors equally contributed to preparing this article.

Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments
We thank all the participants who contributed samples to the study.

References
Abdolmohamadi, K., Alizadeh, H., Ghadiri, F., Taiebli, M., & Fathi, A. (2018). [Psychometric properties of behavioral rating scale of executive functions (BRIEF) in children aged 6 to 12 years (Persian)]. Quarterly of Educational Measurement, 8(30), 135-151. [DOI:10.22054/JEM.2018.24457.1596]
Abdolmohammadi, K., Ghadiri, F., Alizadeh, H., Khodaei, R., & Norouzi, S. (2019). [Comparison of executive functions in preterm and normal children (Persian)]. Journal of North Khorasan University of Medical Sciences, 11(2), 92-97. [Link]
Abdolmohamadi, k., Ashouri, A., Ghadiri Sourman Abadi, F., & Mohammadzadeh, A. (2020). [Prediction of executive functions based on impairment in motor and linguistic growth (Persian)]. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, 26(2), 188-199. [DOI:10.32598/ijpcp.26.2.227.12]
Adi-Japha, E., Berberich-Artzi, J., & Libnawi, A. (2010). Cognitive flexibility in drawings of bilingual children. Child Development, 81(5), 1356–1366. [DOI:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01477.x] [PMID]
Antón, E., Carreiras, M., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2019). The impact of bilingualism on executive functions and working memory in young adults. Plos One, 14(2), e0206770. [PMID] [PMCID]
Antoniou, M., Liang, E., Ettlinger, M., & Wong, P. C. M. (2015). The bilingual advantage in phonetic learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,18(4), 683-695 [DOI:10.1017/S1366728914000777]
Antoniou, M. (2019). The advantages of bilingualism debate. Annual Review of Linguistics, 5, 395-415. [DOI:10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011718-011820]
Arizmendi, G. D., Alt, M., Gray, S., Hogan, T. P., Green, S., & Cowan, N. (2018). Do bilingual children have an executive function advantage? Results from inhibition, shifting, and updating tasks. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 49(3), 356-378. [PMID] [PMCID]
Baker, C. (2006). Foundation of bilingual education and bilingualism Clevedon. England: Multilingual Matters.
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Development, 81(1), 326–339. [PMID] 
Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233-262. [PMID] [PMCID]
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). Individual development and evolution: Experiential canalization of self-regulation. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 647-657. [PMID] 
Carlson, S.M., Zelazo, P.D., & Faja, S. (2013). Executive function. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology, Vol. 1: Body and Mind (pp. 706-743). Oxford: Oxford University Press [DOI:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199958450.013.0025]
Chung-Fat-Yim, A., Sorge, G. B., & Bialystok, E. (2017). The relationship between bilingualism and selective attention in young adults: Evidence from an ambiguous figures task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 70(3), 366–372. [PMID] 
Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge. https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/mono/10.4324/9780203771587/statistical-power-analysis-behavioral-sciences-jacob-cohen
Crespo, K., Gross, M., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2019). The effects of dual language exposure on executive function in Spanish-English bilingual children with different language abilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 188, 104663. [DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104663]
Daller, M., & Ongun, Z. (2018). The Threshold Hypothesis revisited: Bilingual lexical knowledge and non-verbal IQ development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22(6), 675-694. [DOI:10.1177/1367006917690835]
Delphi, M., Delphi, V., Zarifian, T., & Bakhshi, E. (2018). [Comparing phonological processes in Persian-Arabic bilingual children and monolingual children (Persian)]. Journal of Mazandaran University of Medical Sciences, 28 (165), 140-149. [Link]
DeLuca, V., Rothman, J., Bialystok, E., & Pliatsikas, C. (2020). Duration and extent of bilingual experience modulate neurocognitive outcomes. NeuroImage, 204, 116222. [PMID] 
Devine, R. T., Ribner, A., & Hughes, C. (2019). Measuring and predicting individual differences in executive functions at 14 months: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 90(5), e618-e636. [PMID] [PMCID]
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 64, 135-168. [PMID] [PMCID]
Fiske, A., & Holmboe, K. (2019). Neural substrates of early executive function development. Developmental Review: DR, 52, 42–62. [DOI:10.1016/j.dr.2019.100866] [PMID] [PMCID] 
Fogel, Y., Rosenblum, S., Hirsh, R., Chevignard, M., & Josman, N. (2020). Daily performance of adolescents with executive function deficits: An empirical study using a complex-cooking task. Occupational Therapy International, 2020, 3051809. [PMID] [PMCID] 
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to The Study of The Nervous System and Behavior, 86, 186–204. [PMID] [PMCID] 
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Test review behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology, 6(3), 235-238. 
Halse, K.M. (2014). Stability and Predictors of Change in Executive Functions from the Age of 6 to 8 [MA thesis.] Høgskoleringen: Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology. [Link]
Krizman, J., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2016). Bilingual enhancements have no socioeconomic boundaries. Developmental Science, 19(6), 881-891. [PMID]
Kubota, M., Chevalier, N., & Sorace, A. (2020). Losing access to the second language and its effect on executive function development in childhood: The case of ‘returnees. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 55 , 100906. [DOI:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100906]
Last, B. S., Lawson, G. M., Breiner, K., Steinberg, L., & Farah, M. J. (2018). Childhood socioeconomic status and executive function in childhood and beyond. PLoS One. 13(8), e0202964. [PMID] [PMCID]
Lee, H., & Kim, K.H. (2011). Can speaking more languages enhance your creativity? Relationship between bilingualism and creative potential among Korean American students with multicultural link. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(8), 1186-90 [DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2011.01.039]
Memisevic, H., & Sinanovic, O. (2013). Executive functions as predictors of visual-motor integration in children with intellectual disability. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 117(3), 913-22. [DOI:10.2466/15.25.PMS.117x25z4] [PMID]
Memisevic, H. (2015). Self-regulation in children with intellectual disability. Journal of Special Education and Rehabilitation, 16(3-4), 71-83. [Link]
Müller, U., Baker, L., & Yeung, E. (2013). A developmental systems approach to executive function. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 45, 39-66. [PMID]
Nichols, E. S., Wild, C. J., Stojanoski, B., Battista, M. E., & Owen, A. M. (2020). Bilingualism affords no general cognitive advantages: a population study of executive function in 11,000 people. Psychological Science, 31(5), 548–567. [PMID] 
Olulade, O. A., Jamal, N. I., Koo, D. S., Perfetti, C. A., LaSasso, C., & Eden, G. F. (2016). Neuroanatomical evidence in support of the bilingual advantage theory. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991), 26(7), 3196–3204. [PMID] 
Ortega, L. (2019). The study of heritage language development from a bilingualism and social justice perspective. Language Learning, 70(1), 15-53. [DOI:10.1111/lang.12347]
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2016). Should the search for bilingual advantages in executive functioning continue? Cortex; A Journal Devoted to The Study of The Nervous System and Behavior, 74, 305–314. [PMID]
Purić, D., Vuksanović, J., & Chondrogianni, V. (2017). Cognitive advantages of immersion education after 1 year: Effects of amount of exposure. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 296-309. [PMID] 
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Lalwani, L. N., & Velez-Uribe, I. (2015). The effect of language proficiency on executive functions in balanced and unbalanced Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism Language and Cognition, 19, 489-503. [DOI:10.1017/S1366728915000309]
Salvatierra, J.L., & Rosselli, M. (2011). The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15(1), 26-37. [DOI:10.1177/1367006910371021]
Sosic-Vasic, Z., Kröner, J., Schneider, S., Vasic, N., Spitzer, M., & Streb, J. (2017). The association between parenting behavior and executive functioning in children and young adolescents. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 472-481. [DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00472]
Woumans, E., & Duyck, W. (2015). The bilingual advantage debate: Moving toward different methods for verifying its existence. Cortex, 73, 356-357. [PMID]
Type of Study: Research | Subject: Cognitive behavioral
Received: 2022/02/8 | Accepted: 2022/07/23 | Published: 2022/07/24

Add your comments about this article : Your username or Email:
CAPTCHA

Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb